CEHD News Chloe Herzog

CEHD News Chloe Herzog

Update from the Women and Infants Study of Health, Emotions, and Stress (WISHES)

By Colleen Doyle and Erica Smolinski

What is the goal of the WISHES study?

No pregnancy is stress free, although prenatal stress can manifest in different ways for different women. From a research perspective, prenatal stress is a complex umbrella term that encompasses many experiences, including frustration with daily hassles, symptoms of anxiety or depression, and life circumstances such as financial concerns, the death of a loved one, or a disaster like flooding. Overall, prenatal stress includes experiences that drive us “N.U.T.S.”, in that these things are Novel, Unpredictable, Threatening to our survival or our sense of self, and they foster a Sense of lacking control. This acronym isn’t meant to make light of prenatal stress, but rather to help us remember that prenatal stress occurs when a woman has more things coming at her than she can manage.

A growing body of research has linked different levels of these different types of “prenatal stress” experiences to both positive and negative outcomes for women and their developing children. For example, mild levels of prenatal stress have been linked to enhanced motor and cognitive development in infancy. In contrast, more intense or chronic experiences of prenatal stress have been associated with increased risk for an earlier birth, as well as problems with learning and controlling emotions during childhood.

The mechanisms that link women’s experiences during pregnancy to long-term child outcomes are complicated and not completely understood. However, recent research suggests that prenatal stress might influence child outcomes by impacting brain development before birth. The goal of the WISHES study is to increase our understanding in this area. We hope to learn more about how women cope with stress during pregnancy, and how their experiences during pregnancy may “get under the skin” of their developing children to influence their brain development, behavior, and health. We think our study has the potential to make important contributions to how parents, pediatricians, and policy makers can help set up lifelong trajectories of health and well-being by supporting women’s mental and physical health during pregnancy.

What’s involved in participating in the WISHES study?

A photo of a WISHES participant during a fetal monitoring session. Electrodes placed on pregnant women’s bellies allow us to measure fetal heart rate indices that reflect central nervous system development and help us learn how prenatal stress experiences may impact a baby’s brain development before birth.

To study the question of how prenatal stress may influence the health and development of women and their children, the WISHES study is following women and their children from early in pregnancy through the first few months of life. Women enroll in the study between 8-16 weeks of pregnancy, and complete questionnaires on stress, emotions, and health behaviors 5 times during pregnancy and 1 time after pregnancy. At 4 time points during pregnancy, women also complete fetal monitoring sessions, which involve placing electrodes on the woman’s belly to measure her baby’s resting heart rate.

We look at fetal heart rate because it is a “downstream” marker of fetal brain maturation; as central nervous system development unfolds during pregnancy the brain increasingly controls the heart, and in turn resting heart rate patterns show expected patterns of organization and change. Therefore, by measuring changes in resting fetal heart rate during pregnancy we are able to understand how prenatal experiences may play a role in setting up different trajectories of brain development.

At 3 time points during pregnancy and 1 time point following pregnancy, women also provide a small hair sample, which allows us to measure cortisol production during pregnancy. Cortisol is a hormone that helps our body cope and respond in challenging situations. During pregnancy, cortisol also helps mature fetal tissues, such as the lungs, and may impact the development of the central nervous system and brain. Finally, at 2 time points women complete a short computer game while we track their eye-movements, in order to understand how differences in attentional styles may play a role in whether or how women experience stress during pregnancy.

Study enrollment and data collection

So far, 81 pregnant women have enrolled in the WISHES study. Recruitment is ongoing, and our goal is to include up to 120 women. To date, 32 women and their children have completed all visits. As data collection is also ongoing, at this time we are not yet able to report on any significant findings. However, preliminary results suggest that approximately 15-20% of participants report clinically significant levels (i.e., levels that impact their day-to-day functioning) of depression or anxiety at one or more pre- or postnatal study visits. This aligns with prevalence rates of previous studies examining prenatal stress. For us, it also underscores an important point that is often overlooked in this area of research – prenatal stress is not exclusive to the prenatal period. This is important because it means that researchers, health care providers, and policy makers – not to mention partners, family members, and friends – have many opportunities during pregnancy and after delivery to help and support women and their children.

Join us for the WISHES study!

Research suggests that approximately 15-20% of women experience a “clinically significant level of prenatal stress at some point during the perinatal period, a finding that we are seeing replicated in the WISHES study so far.

Every day we are learning more about how experiences during pregnancy may play a role in a child’s growth and development. However, we have much more to learn. This WISHES study will be the first to connect the dots between prenatal experiences, brain development across pregnancy, and brain and behavioral development during the first two years of life. This will allow us to significantly advance our understanding of how prenatal experiences may play a role in setting up different trajectories of brain and behavioral development.

Women and families can be compensated up to $210 for participating. For many of the prenatal visits we are able to meet with women at their homes. For all visits held at the U of M, we provide free parking and child care for older siblings. For more information on the study please email us at wishes.umn@gmail.com.

Gunnar Lab and the Research Network on Toxic Stress and Health

Dr. Gunnar is honored to be part of the Research Network on Toxic Stress and Health, a group with sites throughout the United States in collaboration with the American Academy of Pediatrics and the Harvard Center on the Developing Child. As part of the work of this Network, we have led two research studies working with Children’s Hospitals and Clinics of Minnesota: The Toddlers & Parents Study and the Toddler Attention Study, both described below.

Adversity experienced early in life increases the risk for a variety of physical and mental problems accruing over the lifespan. These children are at increased risk for lifelong problems in learning, behavior, and health. Excessive stress may also increase risk for a host of chronic health impairments such as cardiovascular disease, hypertension, diabetes, addictions, and depression. However, not all children exposed to adverse conditions suffer these consequences. There is a clear need for measures to identify children whose health is affected by extreme stress. If addressed early, the impacts of early adversity can be reduced or reversed. Long before children get to school, pediatricians are in a good position to act as sentinels to identify the children and families who need more help.

But how can pediatricians identify the children who need intervention because of excessive stress? The figure shows the pathways through which excessive stress is expected to influence physical and mental health. The network we are part of is working to develop measures of stress, immune and cellular functioning reflecting adjustments to excessive stress and neurocognitive effects sensitive to excessive stress. The Gunnar lab is working on studies of stress biology and neurocognitive functioning.

Figure 5. Pathways of how stress influence physical and mental health.

Toddlers & Parents Study

Our Toddlers and Parents Study was our study of factors influencing stress biology. The Toddlers and Parents Study was conducted in two pediatric community clinics as part of Children’s Hospitals and Clinics of Minnesota.

Cortisol is a hormone that is affected by stress and affects physical and mental development. We studied 190 children as they got either the 12, 15, or 18 month vaccinations. Secure attachment is important in regulating cortisol levels and reactivity. To measure attachment, a researcher followed the children throughout the clinic visit and during a free play session immediately afterwards. The researcher was observing how confidently the child used the parent as a source of security during the visit. Parents provided information about the family’s income, stresses, and the parents psychological functioning.

Figure 6. Cortisol responses at 1 – clinic arrival, 2 – after physical exam, and 3 – after immunizations.

Some of the families attending these clinics were struggling with poverty and the results showed that poverty was associated with the toddler producing higher levels of cortisol overall during the well child appointments if the toddler was insecurely attached to the parent, but not if the relationship was secure. Poverty in Figure 6 is defined as being below 150% of the federal poverty limit (FPL). All of the children showed a cortisol response to the inoculations, which would be expected. But if you notice, the securely attached children, regardless of family income, show a decrease in cortisol or stress during the physical exam, while the insecurely attached children show an increase or start high and stay high in producing cortisol during the physical exam period. This suggests that they are not able to use the presence of the parent to buffer or lower stress during this part of the clinic visit.

These findings tell us that we probably need to measure both the stressors in the children’s lives and the protective factors, like a secure relationship with their parents, in order to really identify the children in need of additional support. This could be challenging in a quick pediatric visit.

Toddler Attention Study

Regulation attention is a facet of neurocognitive functioning that is very sensitive to chronic stress. But there are no good and easily deployed measures of executive attention regulation that are available before age two. Because regulating attention is so critical to self-control and capable functioning at school, early detection is really important. It is especially important because with early detection there are likely many interventions that could work improve children’s executive attention skills.

The goals of the Toddler Attention Study are to determine whether infants/toddlers who experience stress in their families may demonstrate delays in the development of executive function, social orienting, and joint attention skills and whether these possible delays in attention development can be effectively assessed in the pediatric clinic setting using new tools.

We have been working with colleagues in the Institute of Child Development who are developing tasks that use eye tracking to determine what babies are thinking and how they are processing information. Our goal is to figure out whether these tasks can be deployed on a tablet and used in the midst of a pediatric visit. And if we can get good data in that setting, we want to know whether the scores we are seeing for the children reflect chronic levels of family stress or not.

This study is underway and we have currently completed 102 appointments with families of 9 month-olds (n=38), 12 month-olds (n=43), or 15 month-olds (n=21) at their well-child visits. We expect to complete this study by next winter and hope to share results with you in next year’s newsletter!

Transition into the Family – Peer Follow-up Study

By Carrie DePasquale

As we have mentioned before from the Transition into the Family study, children adopted internationally tend to have an under-active stress-regulatory system and produce too little of the stress hormone cortisol. These children who produce too little cortisol may also be more likely to have difficulties with peers upon entering kindergarten. The Peers follow-up study examined whether this problem persists into middle childhood, when the children are 8-11 years old, because this is when children have increasing independence and peer interactions become more complex. For this study, 58 children from the original Transition into the Family study were paired with another same-age, same-sex child to play several cooperative games. During these games, we measured cortisol production, interaction quality, positive affect, and conflict.

Last year we told you that internationally adopted children seem to be doing just as well as their non-adopted peers during these interactions. This year, after nearly doubling the number of participants, we were encouraged to see that this pattern remains. As you can see in Figure 3, internationally adopted children (the “PI” group) interacted just as well, showed just as much positive affect, and had just as little conflict as their non-adopted peers (the “NA” group). This finding is promising, as it shows that internationally adopted children can improve socially over time.

Figure 3. Average behavior scores during a social interaction with a new peer for children adopted internationally (PI) and non-adopted children (NA).

However, we looked a little deeper and found something very interesting. It seems that, even though children adopted internationally interact with peers just as well as non-adopted children, at least according to our scoring, the way children respond to internationally-adopted children may be different. Figure 4 shows that regardless of whether internationally adopted children were showing positive or negative affect, their peer tended to respond more negatively to them. Conversely, when non-adopted children were positive, their peer was positive, and when they were negative, their peer was negative. This was true of their cortisol production as well. This may mean that there are certain social cues in these interactions that peers are picking up on that we have not. So, while internationally-adopted children’s social behavior seems to improve over time, there may be some nuances of these peer interactions that we still do not understand.

Figure 4. Levels of child positive affect when interacting with either an internationally-adopted child (“PI”) or a non-adopted child (“NA”). “Target child positive affect” indicates the child from the Transition into the Family study, whether internationally adopted or not. “Peer positive affect” refers to the peer they were paired with. As you can see, peer positive affect was more responsive to NA children’s affect. With NA children, peer affect was positive when they were positive and negative when they were negative. With PI children, peer affect was unrelated to the PI child’s affect.

The results of this study are reminiscent of a study we reported on several years ago conducted by Clio Pitula (now a licensed clinical psychologist and assistant professor at Children’s Hospital Colorado). In the BDNF Study, Dr. Pitula examined parent-reports of peer rejection among internationally-adopted teens and found greater reported rejection, even though parents reported similar social behavior towards peers. She also reported that peer rejection for PI teens, as for NA teens, was predictive of higher depressive symptoms. Thus, both of these studies together indicate that we need to better understand the social cues that internationally-adopted, orphanage-reared children are sending that lead to poor peer responses. If you or your children have ideas about this, we would love to hear them. Feedback can be directed to iap@umn.edu.

IN THE COMMUNITY: Greater Minneapolis Crisis Nursery

Carrie Depasquale, Ph.D student, partners with Crisis Nursery implementing stress-reduction techniques

The Greater Minneapolis Crisis Nursery, located in South Minneapolis, provides 72-hour crisis care for children aged birth-6 years in Hennepin County. This means that parents encountering a crisis can bring their children to the Crisis Nursery for 3 days of overnight care, ensuring that their children are safe and cared for while they manage the crisis.

This is an immensely important resource available to families who have inadequate social and economic support systems, while also buffering children from experiencing the worst of the crisis. To that end, the Crisis Nursery works tirelessly to constantly improve their program and train staff to make the experience as positive as possible for children.

Most children who use the Nursery have experienced significant stress and trauma. That is why Dr. Gunnar and her graduate student, Carrie DePasquale, have partnered with the Nursery to implement stress-reduction techniques inspired by mindfulness theory to better help children manage strong emotions and promote self-regulation while at the Nursery. Over the past year, staff have rated these techniques as effective in calming/reducing stress 89% of the time! The ultimate goal will be to assess improvements in child behavior, and then connect with parents to teach them these strategies as well. This may promote more long-term benefits to children’s development in the context of adversity.

The work in the Crisis Nursery was partly inspired by earlier studies conducted by Dr. Gunnar’s former graduate students, Jamie Lawler (now a professor at Eastern Michigan State University) and Elisa Esposito (now a professor at Widener University, Pennsylvania). Results from the Self-Control Study showed that a 6 week summer class in mindfulness-based training for internationally adopted school-aged children 6-10 years, helped them regulate better and exhibit more prosocial behavior according to teacher’s report.

Want to use mindfulness-based techniques with your children? Try taking deep “belly breaths” with your child and doing a body scan. While breathing, draw attention to each part of their body (from their nose to their toes!) and ask your child how it feels.

Interested in donating or volunteering at the Crisis Nursery? Visit their website at https://www.crisisnursery.org/

Pubertal Stress Recalibration Study

By Carrie DePasquale, Nicole Perry, Anna Parenteau, and Megan Gunnar

The Puberty Study reached final enrollment of 321 participants. We retained 85% of families through year two of assessments and about 70% of families have returned for year 3 assessments. There are about 10% of families that have yet to complete year three assessments.

Evidence of Recalibration of Stress System Functioning

The primary reason for conducting the Puberty Study was to determine whether the stress system, first trained under the conditions of orphanage/institutional rearing, would recalibrate its functioning around puberty now that the child was living in supportive, well-resourced homes. We examined this by looking at morning cortisol levels, which so far have not shown evidence of pubertal recalibration with late adopted children showing more blunted morning levels of cortisol, and with children’s cortisol stress hormone responses to a public speaking and arithmetic task.

We have now examined the children’s responses to the public speaking task the first time they did it. As seen in Figure 2, our results showed us, as we expected, that children adopted internationally from orphanages show a blunted cortisol stress response if they are earlier in pubertal development. This is consistent with our evidence in previous studies and evidence collected by colleagues studying children in Romanian orphanages. Studies of children experiencing trauma have also shown this type of blunting of physiological stress reactivity.

Notably, however, for children adopted internationally from orphanages who were screened as in later stages of pubertal development, we saw no difference in cortisol stress response to the public speaking task. This was our prediction and the reason for the study, thus it was gratifying to see that puberty may open up a window for “normalizing” the stress system. Of course, we need to verify this by seeing whether we  can examine the same children as they move through pubertal development to know if we can  see the same shift in stress functioning. Stay tuned.

Emotion Regulation

The ability to regulate negative emotions is a critical skill. Children who are unable to regulate anger and frustration are more likely to lack behavioral control and engage in aggression and defiance. Ineffective regulation of fear and anxiety can often lead to rumination over the source of distress or the suppression of negative affect, both underlying anxiety and depression. The inability to down-regulate negative emotions also makes engaging in socially appropriate interactions with peers and teachers more difficult and it hinders children’s ability to engage with increasingly challenging academic and social tasks that occur as children progress through the school context. Children who have experienced early adversity have been found to struggle in school, have difficulty with friends, and display more behavior problems. Thus, problems in emotion regulation have been described as possibly underlying many of their difficulties.

Emotion regulation develops most rapidly in the first few years of life. A responsive and supportive caregiving environment plays a key role in the development of emotion regulation such that through repeated interactions, emotion regulation skills that were once facilitated by caregiver support are thought to become engrained in the child’s own self-regulatory skillset. Experiencing adversity early in life in the form of institutionalized care may contribute to deficits in emotion regulation later in life.

In the Puberty Study, we hypothesized that children and adolescents who were internationally adopted after being in institutional care would have fewer emotion regulation skills during a social stress test than their non-adopted peers. Further, we were interested in the physiological cost of regulation. We hypothesized that a cortisol response to social stress may be helpful but a biologically expensive tool for regulation. Finally, we tested whether emotion regulation skills were associated with academic, behavioral, and social adjustment in the same way for children experiencing early life adversity and their non-adopted peers.

In this study, children completed the speech portion of the Trier Social Stress Test (TSST). Trained research staff watched videos of the children’s speech and rated the extent to which they were able to regulate their anxiety and engage in the speech. Cortisol was collected at intervals before and after the speech to assess cortisol reactivity to social stress, and parents reported on children’s adjustment.

Results indicated that at younger ages, children who experienced early institutional care did not differ from non-adopted children in their emotion regulation abilities. However, by age 13, children who experienced early life adversity were on average observed to struggle more with regulating their emotions than their non-adopted peers. That is, their skills during the speech did not increase with age as much as those of their non-adopted peers. This suggests that all children show increasing emotion regulation skills with development, but children experiencing early adversity may lag behind, resulting in an increasing gap in emotion regulation at later ages.

We also found that greater emotion regulation skills were associated with an increased cortisol response during the speech for children experiencing early adverse care, while greater emotion regulation was associated with a lower cortisol response for non-adopted children. Thus, when children who experienced early institutional care are able to regulate despite their anxiety, it may come at a higher physiological cost.

We also examined the relations between regulating emotion during our speech task and children’s emotional and behavioral problems according to parents. Here wound no association for the non-adopted comparison children, but a significant association for the adopted children. Specifically, for them being able to regulate despite anxiety was associated with few parent-reported behavior problems.

In conclusion, we may foster the development of children who have had adverse early life histories by helping those who struggle to control their behavior and anxiety to better regulate their emotions; this may be particularly important during adolescence, a time of increasing social and academic challenges, if children who experienced early institutional care start to fall behind their non-adopted peers. We should also recognize, however, that controlling and regulating their emotions may tax them more biologically than children who have not experienced early adversity.

Thank you from our Nurses

We want to express our appreciation to the children and their families for the opportunity to be part of their dedicated participation in the important research of pubertal development at the University of Minnesota these past 4 years. What a privilege it has been to be part of your journey of great change and experience the courage, commitment, and contributions of time and trust in this study that will have meaningful and positive impact for children in the future.

Wishing you all the very best – Jan Goodwalt, RN, Terri Jones, RN, Melissa Stoll, RN.

Math is harder when you’re being watched

By Max Herzberg

Each of us responds to stress in different and unique ways. How these individual differences arise between people is yet to be fully understood. Sometimes the stressful circumstances we encounter are very intense—things like dangerous accidents—but often the stressors we experience in our everyday lives—like giving a short speech at school—are much milder. Even in these mildly stressful situations, however, our bodies respond by releasing a hormone called cortisol to help us face them. According to prior research in adults, only 60% of people produce additional cortisol when they experience social stress, while the other 40% do not. We believe that this difference may be, in part, due to differences in the patterns of brain activation in adolescents who produce a cortisol response compared to those who do not.

Over the past year, our research group has developed a new task to collect cortisol samples during a stressful task while youth have their brain’s scanned. After a preliminary set of pilot data in a simulated brain scan, we began collecting data at the Center for Magnetic Resonance Research, one of the best brain imaging centers in the country. To date, more than 20 adolescents, from 11 – 14 years old, have come to the University to participate in a brain imaging session. All of our participants are asked to lay down in the scanner and give a speech about themselves while two judges watch and appear to rate their performance on a closed-circuit TV connection from another room. In addition to giving the speech, participants complete a set of multiple-choice math problems on a screen in front of them, once while the judges watch and appear to rate them, and once when there is no one watching their performance where no one knows their scores. Throughout the session we record the participant’s brain activity to see how their brains react to giving a speech and doing math with and without the pressure of judges watching them.

Our results suggest that, like adults, not all adolescents produce cortisol in response to the speech and math—it is almost a 60% to 40% split in youth as well. These groups are often called “Responders” and “Non-Responders,” respectively. Further, participants showed different patterns of brain activation when they did the math task with the judges compared to without the judges, regardless of their cortisol responses. Figure 1 shows this difference in brain activation: youth displayed more activation in the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC; Figure 1a) and dorsal lateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC; Figure 1b) when being judged compared to when they were not being judged. These brain regions are often associated with maintaining attention and regulating emotions—two things that are likely important to performing well under pressure.

When we finish our data collection we will have the data to ask two key questions: 1) What is the difference between stress responders and stress non-responders? Are the non-responders doing worse on the math problem? Do they do worse on the speech? Or are they doing as well as the responders, but with less physiological effort? 2) What are the differences in brain activity for responders and non-responders? Are the non-responders better at keeping their fear system in check, for example, more control over the amygdala? Answers to these questions should help us identify ways of helping children who have significant performance anxiety do better on tasks that require them to perform in front of others.

To help us learn more about adolescent’s brain response to stress, we encourage families with children 11-14 years old to participate in the Minnesota Imaging Stress Test in Children (MISTiC) Study. Please contact Max Herzberg at herzb020@umn.edu.

Figure 1. Greater brain activation in the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and dorsal lateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) when completing math problems under pressure compared to completing math problems without pressure.

Early Life Stress, Growth, and Metabolic Health Study

By Brie ReidWith the help of 30 internationally-adopted youth (ages 9 to 18 years) and 90 youth who were matched to each internationally-adopted youth by age and body mass index (weight relative to height), we examined  metabolic and cardiovascular health with state-of-the-art technology.

Many internationally adopted children arrive in their families growth-delayed, with a significant percentage meeting criteria for height stunting (< 5% height-for-age). After adoption, most children experience catch-up growth and soon fall within age norms for height and weight.

Research suggests that the initial stunting and rapid catch-up growth can increase a child’s risk for later physical health problems. Researchers think that this is the case because in the first 1,000 days after conception, young, growing bodies determine whether the environment they are growing up in is rich or poor in nutritional resources. In this way, our bodies “calibrate” to the environment we expect to grow up in. So early physical adaptations (like height-stunting) to environments with very few resources may increase risk for obesity, metabolic syndrome, and cardiovascular disease for children who shift from resource-poor to resource-rich environments.

You might remember our earlier findings in the Puberty Study, where we found that adopted youth who were height stunted were not at an increased risk of obesity. Our group wanted to be sure that we were properly assessing all of the aspects of metabolic and cardiovascular health that are relevant to internationally-adopted children and teens. To do that, we teamed up with Dr. Aaron Kelly in the Department of Pediatrics, who specializes in cardiovascular and metabolic health, and Dr. Don Dengel in the School of Kinesiology, who specializes in high-tech measurements where fat is deposited in the body.

What did we measure?

Body composition

We measured total body fat and visceral adipose tissue (the fat around the gut) with dual X-ray absorptiometry, which was a special x-ray tool that gave us an inside look at how much fat and where fat was located. This tool also helped us measure bone mineral density. One can be thin, but still store a larger percentage of fat in the gut which increases cardiovascular risk.

Cardiovascular Health

In addition to blood pressure, we also measured arterial stiffness and heart rate variability. Arterial stiffness refers to how flexible our arteries are – we want flexible arteries because our heart has a lot of variability in how much blood it needs to pump through our body, less when we are at rest and much more when we exert ourselves physically. When our arteries are flexible, our heart can do its best job pumping blood around our body. Heart rate variability refers to the variation in the time interval between heartbeats.  A healthy heart at rest has higher variability between heart beats.

Diet & Blood Tests

Participants also filled out a food diary so we could understand how their diet might play a role in their health. We also took a blood sample to measure blood lipids, fasting glucose, and fasting insulin.

What did we find?

Compared to non-adopted children of the same age and body mass, youth adopted internationally from orphanages or institutions with a history of mild to severe height-stunting at adoption:

  • Were NOT more likely to have more overall body fat
  • Were more likely to have higher bone mineral density, but both groups had bone mineral density within normal and healthy ranges for the age group.

Versus comparison youth, youth adopted internationally with a history of height-stunting at adoption also:

  • Were more likely to have early indications of arterial stiffness, even though they were relatively lean like our comparison youth.
  • Were more likely to have a higher proportion of trunk fat.
  • Were more likely to have less lean mass overall and less lean mass in the hips, buttocks and thighs.
  • Were more likely to have higher systolic blood pressure.
  • Were more likely to have higher total cholesterol triglycerides, LDL cholesterol, and insulin.

These findings above suggest that youth adopted internationally with a history of height stunting might be at an increased risk of cardiovascular and metabolic issues later in life.

What we recommend

If your child had a history of height-stunting at adoption, we recommend talking with your health care provider about cardiovascular and metabolic health. Many of our participants had a normal BMI, and it is not typical for health care providers to screen for cardiovascular and metabolic health concerns when a young patient is not overweight or obese. Our results do not mean that your child is unhealthy, or has cardiovascular or metabolic health issues. Our results suggest that this might be something for your child to keep track of as they grow older, and talking with your child’s health care provider is the best next step.

ICD graduate student awarded 2018 Women’s Philanthropic Leadership Circle award

Headshot of Michelle Brown
Michelle Brown

Michelle Brown, a graduate student in the Institute of Child Development (ICD), recently received a 2018 Women’s Philanthropic Leadership Circle (WPLC) award. WPLC is a collaborative community that aims to support and enhance the lives of women leaders in the College of Education and Human Development. Brown was one of 11 students awarded.

Each spring, WPLC awards several graduate students up to $2,500 in financial support to honor their achievements. Financial awards are granted to students who demonstrate leadership and creativity through their academics, research, and service.

Brown’s research focuses on childhood adversity and resilience. Ultimately, she hopes to inform researchers, advocates, and social support networks about the tools they can use to help victimized children and adolescents.

CEHD Connect magazine highlights ICD research on trust and learning

Melissa Koenig
Melissa Koenig, Ph.D.
Sarah Suárez
Sarah Suárez

A recent article in CEHD Connect magazine discussed research conducted by the Institute of Child Development (ICD) that focuses on how children develop skills to form trust and learn from others.

The article highlights work by Melissa Koenig, Ph.D., a professor in ICD, and Sarah Suárez, a doctoral student in ICD’s child psychology program. Koenig and Suárez conduct their research as part of the Early Language and Experience Lab, which Koenig directs. In the lab, they aim to understand how children acquire knowledge from others and how they balance the benefits of learning with the risk of misinformation.

“We’re trying to correct a longstanding, flawed, picture of child learners. Children aren’t just accepting whatever they’re told,” Koenig says. “Once you put aside the model of children being passive and credulous learners, it allows you to ask all kinds of questions about how we can support their evaluation of other people and the information they provide.”

To learn more about Koenig and Suárez’s research, read the full story, “Trusting to Learn.”

ICD graduates receive 2018 CEHD Rising Alumni award

Angela Narayan

Angela Narayan, an alumna of the Institute of Child Development (ICD), is a 2018 recipient of the College of Education and Human Development (CEHD) Alumni Society’s Rising Alumni award.

The award recognizes CEHD alumni each year who have achieved distinction early in their career, demonstrate leadership, or show exceptional volunteer service.

Narayan earned her Ph.D. in child psychology from ICD in 2015. She now is an assistant professor of clinical child psychology at the University of Denver, where she also directs the PROTECT Lab, which studies the transmission of parenting behaviors and family dynamics through multiple generations. In her career, Narayan has focused on serving communities in need, as well as families facing poverty, mental health issues, and high levels of stress.

“Words cannot express the pride and delight in mentoring remarkable students like Angela. She exemplifies the passion, intellectual energy, commitment to meaningful research, and community engagement that we cherish in the Institute of Child Development,” said Ann Masten, Ph.D., a Regents Professor, Irving B. Harris Professor of Child Development, and Distinguished McKnight University Professor in ICD. “In April, I had a fun visit to her new lab at the University of Denver, meeting her students and learning more about the exciting work they are doing on risk and resilience in young families. It was a thrill to discuss thorny resilience research issues with the next two generations of developmental scholars.”

Along with Narayan, Korina Barry and Meghan Hickey, who earned undergraduate degrees in child psychology at ICD, were also named Rising Alumni this year. Barry earned a B.A. in child psychology in 2009 and is currently the director of outreach at the University of Minnesota’s Center for Advanced Studies in Child Welfare. Hickey graduated with a B.S. in child psychology in 2004 and now works as a special education supervisor for Robbinsdale Area Schools, where she leads and mentors special education teachers.

ICD graduate students awarded the 2018 National Science Foundation fellowship

Isabella Stallworthy
Isabella Stallworthy
Shreya Lakhan-Pal
Shreya Lakhan-Pal

Isabella Stallworthy and Shreya Lakhan-Pal, graduate students at the Institute of Child Development (ICD), have recently been selected to receive the 2018 National Science Foundation (NSF) Graduate Research Fellowship Program (GRFP) grant. The fellowship is awarded to outstanding graduate students in NSF- supported sciences who are pursuing a research-based master’s or doctoral degree. The NSF Graduate Research Fellowship Program is the country’s oldest fellowship that directly supports students in various science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM) fields.

As a NSF Graduate Research fellow, Stallworthy plans to build on her past and current research focusing on self-regulation and social engagement in early infancy from bio-behavioral and social-cognitive perspectives. Through her research, Stallworthy hopes to inform parent education and caregiver interventions on ways to promote successful socio-emotional, communicative, and self-regulatory skills early in life. “I am excited to build upon my past research experiences to ask new questions about the emergence of the social mind and brain, synthesizing ideas from multiple labs and research traditions,” Stallworthy said.

Lakhan-Pal’s research focuses on the understanding of emotional regulation during the transition to adolescence. With help from the fellowship, Lakhan-Pal plans to use electroencephalography (EEG) to assess whether parenting practices around emotions have an impact on how effective teens are able to self-regulate. “I’m mainly curious on how parents’ supportiveness and tendency to coach kids through emotional experiences will affect their children’s ability to regulate during adolescence,” Lakhan-Pal said. 

Two ICD doctoral students receive Doris Duke Fellowship

Katherine Ridge
Katherine Ridge
Christina Mondi
Christina Mondi

Katherine Ridge and Christina Mondi, doctoral students at the Institute of Child Development (ICD), have recently been awarded the Doris Duke Fellowship for the Promotion of Child Well-Being.

The fellowship, offered by Chapin Hall at the University of Chicago and funded by the Doris Duke Charitable Foundation, aims to identify and develop a new generation of leaders who will create practices and policies that will enhance child development and prevent child maltreatment.

With the help of the fellowship, Ridge plans to investigate how characteristics of early relationships with caregivers influence children’s trusting decisions. In addition to pursuing a Ph.D. in child psychology at ICD, Ridge is also a student in the school psychological services M.A. and specialist certificate program in the Department of Educational Psychology. Ridge hopes to promote the development of positive relationships between children and adults. “With the support of the Doris Duke Fellowship, I am especially excited to use the knowledge gained from our research to inform school-based support groups for children and their relationships with others during my internship year,” Ridge said.

Mondi’s research and clinical work focuses on the promotion of socio-emotional learning with an emphasis placed on populations affected by adversity and trauma. Mondi hopes to better understand the role that early childhood intervention programs, such as the Child-Parent Center P-3 program, have on promoting lifelong wellbeing. As a Doris Duke Fellow, Mondi will use this opportunity to conduct research that will contribute to the growing national conversation about how to promote lifelong mental health. “I look forward to collaborating with and learning from other scholars who are passionate about promoting child wellbeing,” Mondi said.

Fellows receive an annual stipend of $30,000 for up to two years to support their dissertation and related research. Ridge and Mondi are two of 15 doctoral students to receive the fellowship this year.

Masten discusses resilience in Monitor on Psychology

Dr. Ann Masten
Dr. Ann Masten

Ann S. Masten, Ph.D., Regents Professor and Irving B. Harris Professor of Child Development in the Institute of Child Development (ICD), was recently featured in an article appearing in the September 2017 issue of the American Psychological Association’s Monitor on Psychology.

The article, “Maximizing children’s resilience,” by Kirsten Weir, highlighted new research that examines how to foster resilience in children and adolescents and the importance of early intervention.

According to Masten, the field has shifted from focusing on traits of resilient individuals to looking at resilience from a systems perspective. For example, Masten, along with other researchers, have found that having supportive relationships, including with parents or primary caregivers, is important for healthy development.

“The resilience of an individual depends on drawing resources from many other systems,” Masten says. “A child is embedded in interactions with friends, family, community. The way those other systems are functioning plays a huge role in the capacity of that child to overcome adversity.”

ICD alumna, undergraduate featured in Star Tribune for restorative justice project

Rebecca Shlafer

Rebecca Shlafer, Ph.D., MPH, an alumna of the Institute of Child Development (ICD), and Laura Reimann, an undergraduate child psychology student in ICD, were recently featured in the Star Tribune for their involvement in Project Teddy Bear, a restorative justice project at a Federal Correctional Institution in Sandstone, Minn.

Shlafer, who teaches an honors class titled, Incarceration and the Family, partnered with Diana Poch, a psychologist at Sandstone, to launch the project. Poch had noticed positive behavior changes in inmates who learned how to crochet and were teaching others the craft.

Last semester, Shlafer and her students collected a total of 350 pounds of yarn to provide to the inmates. With the yarn, the inmates crocheted animals for sick children at four Twin Cities Ronald McDonald Houses.

“It was so powerful for my students to learn how many consequences there are to sometimes very limited decisions,” Shlafer said. “They made an impact in a way that really challenged the students’ assumptions about who is in prison for what and why, raising questions around equity.”

Reimann plans to continue to raise awareness about Project Teddy Bear next semester as Shlafer’s teaching assistant. “People have a tremendous capacity to change if given the chance and the resources,” Reimann said. “They are creating something with another human in mind and giving something back to a community that thinks they are only taking.”

ICD professor, graduate student present research on mindfulness in education

ICD Professor Philip D. Zelazo delivering a keynote address at the Mindfulness in Education Summer Institute.

Philip D. Zelazo, Ph.D., a Nancy M. and John E. Lindahl Professor in the Institute of Child Development (ICD), and Andrei Semenov, a child psychology doctoral student in ICD, recently presented at the Mindfulness in Education Summer Institute.

The summer institute is a community event hosted by the University of Minnesota’s Center for Spirituality and Healing. The 3-day event aimed to bring together teachers, researchers, clinicians, and practitioners to discuss mindfulness research and ways to promote practices that support wellbeing in school communities.

For the event, Zelazo delivered a keynote address that focused on how mindfulness practice has been shown to promote reflection and executive functions in children and adults.

Semenov’s presentation highlighted findings from curriculum evaluation conducted this past year. The novel curriculum, developed in collaboration with the Center for Spirituality and Healing, introduced mindfulness practice to a cohort of elementary school teachers in an effort to improve teacher wellbeing and promote mindful approaches to student-teacher interactions.

Palmer awarded summer fellowship to research children’s mental health

Alyssa Palmer, a Ph.D. child psychology student in the Institute of Child Development (ICD), was awarded a 2017 Translational Summer Research Fellowship by the Institute for Translational Research in Children’s Mental Health (ITR).

ITR’s primary mission is to bridge the gap between research and practice in children’s mental health.

The fellowship aims to help graduate students pursue collaborative research projects on the development or expansion of evidence-based prevention or treatment interventions in children’s mental health. Palmer is one of four graduate students who was awarded a fellowship this year.

Palmer’s research will focus on parent self-regulation, parenting quality, and child behavioral outcomes in homeless families.

Categories:

Greetings from Professor Megan Gunnar

The Gunnar Lab research team continues to study the impact of early experiences through our work with families formed through international adoption and Minnesota-born families. Much of our work over the past year focused on the development of children, teens and also young adults. I am especially grateful for the work
of Professor Rich Lee and his students in the Familee Lab who work with young adults who were adopted from Korea. Rich’s work helps us understand the experience of international adoption as it relates to identity and the experience of discrimination.

The overall goal of the Gunnar Lab’s research is to study stress and the experiences that shape how the body deals with stress. In this newsletter issue, you can read more about our latest research studies examining stress during pregnancy and its effect on the developing fetus, stress during the transition to adolescence, a very challenging time of life, and stress encountered in social situations where children attempt to meet and work with new peers.

Much of our stress work is taking a medical turn as we evaluate the health and physical development consequences of starting our life in harsh and stressful conditions as is the case for many children adopted internationally from orphanages.

Thank you to all the families who have taken part in our research.

~ Regents Professor Megan Gunnar

Puberty Study: Growth and Pubertal Development

By Brie Reid

Throughout evolution, humans have experienced periods of feast and famine. Growing evidence suggests that adversity and height growth stunting early in childhood increases risk for early onset puberty (in girls), obesity, and poorer mental health later in life. Researchers think that this is the case because in the first 1,000 days after conception, our very young, growing bodies determine whether the environment we are growing up in has a lot of resources or has little resources. In this way, our bodies “calibrate” to the environment we expect to grow up in. However, researchers think that early physical adaptations to harsh environments with very little resources may increase later risks of obesity, early onset puberty in girls and metabolic syndrome and cardiovascular disease, because our bodies do not anticipate a shift from resource-poor to resource-rich environments.

We tested this hypothesis with 283 youth aged 7-14 years who participated in the first year of the Puberty Study. We looked at data collected from adopted children’s first medical clinic visit post-adoption and we also looked at data collected from the nurse’s exam, see Figure 1, in the Puberty Study.

Growth

Figure 1. Nurses measure participant’s growth using different methods.

On average, adopted youth were shorter (by ~1.6”) and had lower BMI-for-age than their non-adopted peers. Adopted youth also tended to have lower numbers of body fat percentage. On average, both groups of youth were within normal ranges of height, weight, and body fat. There were no differences in waist-to-hip ratios or waist-to-height ratios between groups. A key finding was that there were very few overweight or obese youth in either the adopted or the non-adopted group, which surprised us because nearly one in three children ages 10 to 17 are overweight or obese in America.

Pubertal Development

Two graphs comparing BMI for age for adopted and non-adopted youth.

Figure 2. No group difference between adoptees and MN born participants.

We found that pubertal status was determined by being older and being heavier for your age. This is just what is expected as bodies typically put on fat to support the pubertal changes in growth. What surprised us is that we found no evidence that the internationally adopted children, many of whom had experienced relatively harsh conditions in orphanages prior to adoption, were going through puberty earlier than the children born into their Minnesota family as seen in Figure 2. We did not see this for either boys or girls.

Previously Height Stunted Adopted Youth Results

Previously height-stunted youth adopted internationally, though still shorter on average, were not found to be at greater risk for high BMI and were also less likely to be in later puberty stages.

Our conclusion?

Early life adversity and height-growth stunting early in life do not always lead to early puberty or obesity in later childhood or adolescence!

Early removal from adversity or later contexts of highly-resourced homes could protect children from long-term impacts on their BMI and the timing of puberty. Subsequent waves of longitudinal data collection will provide a window into pubertal timing across three years to determine if adopted and previously height-stunted youth experience a different pubertal development tempo than non-adopted peers. This tempo could also impact body fat as our sample of participants get older.

Multiple years of follow-up and more precise measures of where children store fat on their bodies and metabolic health are our next steps to ensure we understand the full picture of growth and pubertal development in the context of early challenging life conditions.

New Study Opportunity: Early Stress, Growth, and Metabolic Health Study

New research suggests that early life stress and early height stunting can contribute to later health by impacting the growth, body fat composition, and cardiovascular health. This may mean that experiences in childhood influence our health in adulthood. If we can identify these changes early on, then we can develop interventions to hopefully prevent later health problems.

In the Puberty Study, we did not find that children who were growth-stunted at adoption were becoming overweight as they approached adolescence, so the question for heart health is actually where does the body put its fat. Deep visceral (belly) fat is a risk factor for Type II diabetes and heart disease. You can be normal weight and yet still be at risk by packing fat in deep belly areas.

To be sure that children who were short for their age at adoption and then grew quickly are also heart and body composition healthy, we will be conducting a study using cutting-edge measures of body fat composition and cardiovascular health measures. Personalized results will be given to the parents of each participant and these can be taken to their pediatrician.

For this study we are looking for children and teenagers (ages 8 through 17 years) who were adopted internationally from orphanages or similar institutions.

In addition to personalized results, participants will be compensated for 1 visit to the University of Minnesota to have a full-body DXA scan of body composition, their cardiovascular health assessed, have blood drawn, and answer questionnaires.

If you would like more information about this study, please feel free to email Brie at reidx189@umn.edu

Parenting Results from Transition Study

Mother lifting baby into the air and smiling

 

By Kalsea Koss and Jamie Lawler

Parents are the most important people in young children’s lives. They comfort them, care for them, and help them learn and grow. Lots of research has shown how parenting affects kids raised in their families of origin, but we wanted to look in particular at parenting in internationally adopting families. We wanted to see how it may be similar or different than other families and to see how parenting affects the recovery children make following adoption. Our team was especially interested in which aspects of parenting might help adopted kids learn to control their behavior and emotions. We also know that parenting is a hard job (maybe the hardest job of all!) and we were curious whether it was more challenging to parent a child who had tougher experiences before adoption. Finally, we wanted to see whether the consistency of family routines, such as meal times and bedtimes, helped children learn better self-control.

What we did:

We examined parenting and the family environment as part of the Transition into the Family Study. If you remember from past newsletters, the Transition Study was a multi-year project following children adopted from institutions, seeing them every eight months for the first several years that they were in their families. These initial years are a period of rapid recovery from pre-adoption deprivation for children who lived in orphanages or other institutions. To look at parenting, we asked families to come to our research lab to participate in a number of different activities four times over the first two years post-adoption. Activities included playing normally as they would at home, structured play activities (like making a scene using Playdoh), and cleaning up toys together. We also asked parents to complete several daily diaries to get a sense of the typical day for the children at home. To measure how well children recovered following adoption, we looked at how well they could regulate their behavior when asked to transition between tasks at the four laboratory visits, how children performed on a number of tasks designed to challenge their attention and self-control, and asked parents and the child’s kindergarten teachers to complete a number of questionnaires about their behavior and functioning.

Who participated:

145 families, including 93 families who had adopted a child internationally from an orphanage or other institution were part of this portion of the study. We also followed children adopted internationally from foster care situations, but these children and their families are not a part of this analysis.

What we found:

First, we looked at the quality of parenting received by children adopted out of institutions compared with children reared in their birth families. We found that parenting did not differ between the groups on either sensitivity (the parent’s ability to respond effectively to the child’s cues in a way that supports the child, encourages emotional development, and promotes the child’s confidence that their needs will be met) or structure/limit-setting (the parent’s ability to produce predictable experiences and expectations for the child as well as structure the child’s behavior). We also found that both groups demonstrated above average parenting.

Second, we wanted to see whether children’s pre-adoptive care would predict the parenting they received after adoption. We found that children adopted from higher quality pre-adoptive care received higher rates of sensitivity and more effective structure/limit-setting from adoptive parents. This may be because these children were able to provide clearer signals of their needs and wants. We were also curious how child behavior and parenting would affect one another in the first year following adoption and found that child behavior soon after adoption does NOT predict later parenting, but a parent’s ability to provide structure and set limits soon after adoption predicts how children are able to regulate their behavior 8 months later.

Next, we wanted to look past the first year after adoption and see how parenting affects children’s self-control in kindergarten. We found that better parent structure/limit-setting predicted children’s self-control on several different tasks. Children who had received more effective structure and limit-setting from parents were better able to control their impulses when asked to use their words rather than their hands to select a toy prize. They also did better on a computer game that asked them to pay attention to certain things and ignore others. These children also showed better ability to regulate their emotions in kindergarten.

Finally, children who receive poorer preadoption care are more likely to have lasting problems in impulse control and attention regulation. Because of this, we were interested in whether parenting might affect the connection between poor pre-adoptive care and later child functioning. We found that children who received high quality parenting did not show the same lasting effects of poor early care as children who received less ideal parenting. Because parents in our study generally displayed “better than average” parenting, this finding suggests that children who experienced more adversity before adoption needed really good, not just good parenting in order to recover. For example, children whose parents scored really high in their ability to structure the environment and set limits showed good emotion regulation regardless of their pre-adoptive adversity, whereas children whose parents scored in the average range on these aspects of parenting struggled more in regulating their emotions if they had experienced more pre-adoption adversity. Additionally, children in families with very consistent daily routines (for example, the same meal times and bed times each day) who had experienced poor care before adoption showed better ability to wait for a surprise and showed fewer ADHD symptoms than children adopted from similar circumstances whose families had less consistency in routines. Consistent routines did not seem to help with indiscriminate friendliness. In fact, for children adopted from more adverse pre-adoption conditions, greater consistency in routines predicted more indiscriminate friendliness. We are not sure why this might be the case, but it does suggest that stability in routines may help some aspects of recovery from pre-adoption condition but slow others.

Women and Infants Study of Health, Emotions, and Stress (WISHES)

By Colleen Doyle

Midwife Ann Forster Page, a collaborator of the WISHES research project, examines a patient at the Women’s Health Specialists Clinic.

What makes us who we are? Historically, great debate has surrounded this question. Some have said it’s the genes we inherit at conception. Others have pointed to the environment we experience in childhood. Today, there is growing evidence that many of our individual characteristics—including our temperaments, our intelligence, and our mental and physical health—may be influenced by the interplay of our genes and the prenatal environment that we experience before birth. The goal of our new WISHES study (the Women and Infants Study of Health, Emotions, and Stress) is to learn more about how women’s experiences during pregnancy may “get under the skin” of their developing children to influence their brain development, behavior, and health.

Why are we studying prenatal experiences? All women experience some amount of stress and mood swings during pregnancy. However, a growing body of research has linked different levels of these “prenatal stress” experiences to both positive and negative outcomes for women and their developing children. For example, mild levels of prenatal stress have been linked to enhanced motor and cognitive development in infancy. In contrast, more intense or chronic experiences of prenatal stress have been associated with increased risk for an earlier birth, as well as problems with learning and controlling emotions during childhood. The mechanisms that link women’s experiences during pregnancy to long-term child outcomes are complicated and not completely understood. The goal of the WISHES study is to increase our understanding in this area. We think our study has the potential to make important contributions to how parents, pediatricians, and policy makers can help set up lifelong trajectories of health and well-being by supporting women’s health during pregnancy.

 What is prenatal stress? Prenatal stress is a complex umbrella term that encompasses many experiences – from frustration with daily hassles, to mental health concerns related to anxiety or depression, to life circumstances that are difficult or impossible for women to control, such as significant financial concerns, the death of a loved one, or a disaster like flooding.

Additionally, a woman’s perception of whether something is stressful contributes to how she may experience prenatal stress. This means factors like a woman’s personality, her outlook on life, and whether she views her world as safe or unsafe can come into play.

Finally, prenatal stress is not exclusive to the prenatal period. Research shows that women who report feeling stressed, anxious, or depressed during pregnancy also report feeling this way through at least the first two years of their child’s life. This is important to remember, because this means there are potentially many opportunities to support women and their children.

Although the “prenatal stress recipe” can be different for every woman, we might think about the ingredients as being the sort of things that drive us “N.U.T.S.”, in that these things are Novel, Unpredictable, Threatening to our survival or our sense of self, and they foster a Sense of lacking control. This acronym isn’t meant to make light of prenatal stress, but rather to help us remember that prenatal stress occurs when a woman has more things coming at her than she can manage.

How does prenatal stress influence child development? Recent research suggests that experiences of prenatal stress might influence child outcomes by impacting brain development before birth. What we are learning is that prenatal stress can affect a woman’s health during pregnancy in two ways, which in turn can influence her child’s development. First, prenatal stress can affect behaviors that are important to maintaining good health during pregnancy (e.g., getting enough sleep, eating right, exercising, and getting good prenatal care). Second, prenatal stress can affect a woman’s biological functioning during pregnancy, including her hormones, blood pressure, and immune system. The placenta, which regulates the prenatal environment, likely plays a major role here.

What’s involved in the WISHES study? So far, we have very strong evidence from animal research that supports the hypothesis that prenatal stress influences offspring brain development before birth. To study this question in humans, the WISHES study is following women and their children from early in pregnancy through the first two years of life. Women enroll in the study between 8-12 weeks of pregnancy, and complete questionnaires on stress, emotions, and health behaviors 5 times during pregnancy. At 4 time points during pregnancy, women also complete fetal monitoring sessions, which involve placing electrodes on the woman’s belly to measure her baby’s resting heart rate. We look at fetal heart rate because it is a “downstream” marker of fetal brain maturation; as central nervous system development unfolds during pregnancy the brain increasingly controls the heart, and in turn resting heart rate patterns show expected patterns of organization and change. Therefore, by measuring changes in resting fetal heart rate during pregnancy we are able to understand how prenatal experiences may play a role in setting up different trajectories of brain development. At 3 time points during pregnancy, women also provide a small hair sample, which allows us to measure cortisol production during pregnancy. Cortisol is a hormone that helps our body cope and respond in challenging situations. During pregnancy, cortisol also helps mature fetal tissues, such as the lungs, and may impact the development of the central nervous system and brain. Finally, at 1 time point women complete a short computer game while we track their eye-movements, in order to understand how differences in attentional styles may play a role in whether or how women experience stress during pregnancy.

Following delivery, at 3, 6, 9, 12, and 24 months, children complete behavioral assessments and MRI brain scans, and parents complete questionnaires on their own experiences and their child’s development. We use MRI for this study because unlike other imaging methods it does not involve any radiation, making it completely safe for use with infants. Also, all scans are conducted in the evenings during children’s natural sleep, without the use of any sedation. Finally, a staff member is in the scanner with the child throughout the scan, so the scan can be discontinued immediately when children wake up.

Join us for the WISHES study. Every day we are learning more about how experiences during pregnancy may play a role in a child’s growth and development. However, we have much more to learn. This WISHES study will be the first to connect the dots between prenatal experiences, brain development across pregnancy, and brain and behavioral development during the first two years of life. This will allow us to significantly advance our understanding of how prenatal experiences may play a role in setting up different trajectories of brain and behavioral development.

Women and families will be compensated up to $560 for participating and will receive a small gift featuring pictures of their child’s brain. For many of the prenatal visits we are able to meet with women at their homes. For all visits held at the U of M, we provide free parking and child care for older siblings. For more information on the study please email us at doyle291@umn.edu.